CallingBullshit_Chap.3.pdf

6/27/2021 Chapter 3: The Nature of Bullshit, Calling Bullshit

https://web-b-ebscohost-com.ezproxy.losrios.edu/ehost/ebookviewer/ebook/bmxlYmtfXzIyOTMxMDlfX0FO0?sid=3be3e61a-003b-4171-9b17-ba1a469… 1/9

CHAPTER 3

The Nature of Bullshit

S O WHAT IS BULLSHIT, EXACTLY? People sometimes use the term the same way that little kids
use the word “unfair”: to describe anything they don’t like. Used in this way, the term can
apply to a close but correct call at home plate, or a parking ticket written two minutes after
your meter expires. Other times the term applies to injustice: a corrupt process of granting
government contracts, an unwarranted acquittal, or legislation denying people basic civil
liberties.

Then there is language to soften bad news and more generally grease the wheels of social
interaction. “I just loved your turnip lasagna. You absolutely must give me the recipe.” “What
a beautiful baby!” “It’s not you, it’s me.” “Your haircut looks great.” “Your call is important to
us.” “I had a lovely evening.” These are often bullshit, but they’re not really the kind of bullshit
we’re concerned with here.

Insincere promises and outright lies get a bit closer to the mark. “Honey, I’m going to have
to work late again tonight”; “I didn’t inhale”; “I’d love to give you a better deal if I could”; “I
have read and agreed to the above terms and conditions”; “Read my lips: No new taxes.” Still,
we tend to think of these claims as outright lies rather than bullshit.

But lies are often most persuasive when dressed in superfluous details; these details come
quite close to what we mean by “bullshit.” A friend leaves you waiting for twenty-five minutes
at the local café because he got distracted watching YouTube videos and left his house half an
hour late. “I’m sorry I’m late, traffic was terrible. You know that bridge down by Fifteenth? A
bus had stalled right where it goes down to two lanes each way, and some idiot had rear-
ended it, blocking the other lane. So by the time I got through all that mess…” The first
sentence is a lie; the subsequent ones are bullshit.

Harry Frankfurt, the philosopher we introduced in the preface, refined this notion a bit
further. He described bullshit as what people create when they try to impress you or persuade
you, without any concern for whether what they are saying is true or false, correct or
incorrect. Think about a high school English essay you wrote without actually reading the
book, a wannabe modernist painter’s description of his artistic vision, or a Silicon Valley tech
bro co-opting a TED Talk invitation to launch his latest startup venue. The intention may be
to mislead, but it need not be. Sometimes we are put on the spot and yet have nothing to say.
The bullshit we produce under those circumstances is little more than the “filling of space
with the inconsequential.”

Bullshit can be total nonsense. Another philosopher to take up the issue of bullshit, G. A.
Cohen, notes that a lot of bullshit—particularly of the academic variety—is meaningless and
so cloaked in rhetoric and convoluted language that no one can even critique it. Thus for

38

39

6/27/2021 Chapter 3: The Nature of Bullshit, Calling Bullshit

https://web-b-ebscohost-com.ezproxy.losrios.edu/ehost/ebookviewer/ebook/bmxlYmtfXzIyOTMxMDlfX0FO0?sid=3be3e61a-003b-4171-9b17-ba1a469… 2/9

Cohen, bullshit is “unclarifiable unclarity.” Not only is the bullshitter’s prose unclear, but the
ideas underlying it are so ill-formed that it cannot possibly be clarified. Cohen suggests a test
for unclarity: If you can negate a sentence and its meaning doesn’t change, it’s bullshit.
“Shakespeare’s Prospero is ultimately the fulcrum of an epistemic tragedy, precisely because
of his failure to embrace the hermeneutics of the transfinite.”

What these notions of bullshit have in common is that the speaker aims to persuade or
impress, rather than to lead the listener toward the truth. The speaker may do this with active
obfuscation, or simply by talking nonsense to conceal the fact that he or she doesn’t actually
know anything about the subject at hand. Some authors distinguish between persuasive
bullshit and evasive bullshit. The former aims to convey an exaggerated sense of competence
or authority, while the latter avoids directly answering a question that the speaker would
prefer not to address.

When someone is bullshitting, he and his audience are not allies in the process of
communication. Rather, the speaker is aiming to manipulate the listener with rhetorical flair,
superfluous detail, or statistical snake oil. For us:

Bullshit involves language, statistical figures, data graphics, and other forms of
presentation intended to persuade or impress an audience by distracting,
overwhelming, or intimidating them with a blatant disregard for truth, logical
coherence, or what information is actually being conveyed.

The key elements of this definition are that bullshit bears no allegiance to conveying the
truth, and that the bullshitter attempts to conceal this fact behind some type of rhetorical veil.
Sigmund Freud illustrated the concept about as well as one could imagine in a letter he wrote
his fiancée, Martha Bernays, in 1884:

So I gave my lecture yesterday. Despite a lack of preparation, I spoke quite well and
without hesitation, which I ascribe to the cocaine I had taken beforehand. I told about
my discoveries in brain anatomy, all very difficult things that the audience certainly
didn’t understand, but all that matters is that they get the impression that I
understand it.

While he has not written about bullshit directly, the sociologist of science Bruno Latour
has had a formative effect on our thinking about how people bullshit their audiences. Latour
looks at the power dynamics between an author and a reader. In Latour’s worldview, a
primary objective of nonfiction authors is to appear authoritative. One good way to do this is
to be correct, but that is neither necessary nor sufficient. Correct or not, an author can adopt a
number of tactics to make her claims unassailable by her readers—who in turn strive not to be
duped. For example, the author can line up a phalanx of allies by citing other writers who
support her point, or whose work she builds upon. If you question me, she implies, you have
to question all of us. She can also deploy sophisticated jargon. Jargon may facilitate technical
communication within a field, but it also serves to exclude those who have not been initiated
into the inner circle of a discipline.

BULLSHIT AND BLACK BOXES

A

40

41

6/27/2021 Chapter 3: The Nature of Bullshit, Calling Bullshit

https://web-b-ebscohost-com.ezproxy.losrios.edu/ehost/ebookviewer/ebook/bmxlYmtfXzIyOTMxMDlfX0FO0?sid=3be3e61a-003b-4171-9b17-ba1a469… 3/9

A ccording to Latour, scientific claims are typically built upon the output of metaphorical
“black boxes,” which are difficult if not impossible for the reader to penetrate. These black
boxes often involve the use of specialized and often expensive equipment and techniques that
are time-consuming and unavailable, or are so broadly accepted that to question them
represents a sort of scientific heresy.*1 If I were to write a paper claiming that specific genetic
variants are associated with susceptibility to bullshit, a skeptic might reasonably argue my
choice of sample population, the way I measure bullshit susceptibility, or the statistical
method I use to quantify associations. But the biotechnology used to derive the DNA
sequences from blood samples would typically be treated as a black box. In principle a skeptic
could question this as well, but to do so she would be challenging the scientific establishment
and, more important for our purposes, she would need access to advanced equipment and
extensive technical expertise in molecular genetics.

Latour is not saying that these aspects of academic science make the entire enterprise
bullshit, and neither are we. He is saying only that science is more than a dispassionate search
for the truth, a theme to which we will return in chapter 9. The important thing about
Latour’s black box idea is that we see a powerful analogy here to what speakers do when they
bullshit effectively. Outright lies are often straightforward to catch and refute. But effective
bullshit is difficult to fact-check. Bullshit can act like one of Latour’s black boxes, shielding a
claim from further investigation.

Suppose a friend tells you, “You know, on average, cat people earn higher salaries than
dog people.” It’s easy to call bullshit on that statement when it stands by itself. And when you
do so, perhaps your friend will simply laugh and admit, “Yeah, I made that up.”

But suppose instead she doubles down and starts filling out—or making up—details to
support her claim. “No, really, it’s true. I saw this TED Talk about it. They explained how cat
owners value independence whereas dog owners value loyalty. People who value
independence are more likely to have NVT…no…NVS…I can’t remember, but some kind of
personality. And that makes them better able to rise in the workplace.”

This is full-on bullshit, and it functions like one of Latour’s black boxes. If you want to
dispute your friend’s claims, you now have substantial work to do. This is where lies and
bullshit come together: In our view, a lie becomes bullshit when the speaker attempts to
conceal it using various rhetorical artifices.

Now imagine that she points you to a research study that makes this claim. Suppose you
track down the study, and read something like the following:

We observe a statistically significant difference in cat- and dog-lovers’ earnings,
based on an ANCOVA using log-transformed earnings data (F = 3.86).

If you don’t have a professional background in statistics, you’ve just slammed head-on
into a particularly opaque black box. You probably don’t know what an ANCOVA is or what
the F value means or what a log transformation is or why someone would use it. If you do
know some of these things, you still probably don’t remember all of the details. We, the
authors, use statistics on a daily basis, but we still have to look up this sort of stuff all the
time. As a result, you can’t unpack the black box; you can’t go into the details of the analysis
in to pick apart possible problems. Unless you’re a data scientist, and probably even
then, you run into the same kind of problem you encounter when you read about a paper that
uses the newest ResNet algorithm to reveal differences in the facial features of dog and cat
owners. Whether or not this is intentional on the part of the author, this kind of black box
shields the claim against scrutiny.

42

6/27/2021 Chapter 3: The Nature of Bullshit, Calling Bullshit

https://web-b-ebscohost-com.ezproxy.losrios.edu/ehost/ebookviewer/ebook/bmxlYmtfXzIyOTMxMDlfX0FO0?sid=3be3e61a-003b-4171-9b17-ba1a469… 4/9

But it doesn’t need to. The central theme of this book is that you usually don’t have to
open the analytic black box in to call bullshit on the claims that come out of it. Any
black box used to generate bullshit has to take in data and spit results out, like the diagram on
this page.

Most often, bullshit arises either because there are biases in the data that get fed into the
black box, or because there are obvious problems with the results that come out. Occasionally
the technical details of the black box matter, but in our experience such cases are uncommon.
This is fortunate, because you don’t need a lot of technical expertise to spot problems with the
data or results. You just need to think clearly and practice spotting the sort of thing that can
go wrong. In the pages that follow, we will show you how to do precisely this.

RETURNING TO OUR CAT and dog example, instead of digging into the details of the statistical

analysis, you might ask how the samples were collected. Maybe the study looked at pet
ownership and merged data from people in living in New York City—where salaries are high
and keeping a dog is difficult—with data from upstate New York, where salaries are lower and
dogs are far more practical. Maybe dog-lover salaries are assumed to be the US average
whereas cat-lover salaries were solicited from visitors to a website for startup founders who
cohabitate with cats.

If the data that go into the analysis are flawed, the specific technical details of the analysis
don’t matter. One can obtain stupid results from bad data without any statistical trickery. And
this is often how bullshit arguments are created, deliberately or otherwise. To catch this sort
of bullshit, you don’t have to unpack the black box. All you have to do is think carefully about
the data that went into the black box and the results that came out. Are the data unbiased,
reasonable, and relevant to the problem at hand? Do the results pass basic plausibility
checks? Do they support whatever conclusions are drawn?

Being able to spot bullshit based on data is a critical skill. Decades ago, fancy language and
superfluous detail might have served a bullshitter’s needs. Today, we are accustomed to
receiving information in quantitative form, but hesitant to question that information once we
receive it. Quantitative evidence generally seems to carry more weight than qualitative
arguments. This weight is largely undeserved—only modest skill is required to construct
specious quantitative arguments. But we defer to such arguments nonetheless. Consequently,
numbers offer the biggest bang for the bullshitting buck.

CRIMINAL MACHINE LEARNING

L

43

44

6/27/2021 Chapter 3: The Nature of Bullshit, Calling Bullshit

https://web-b-ebscohost-com.ezproxy.losrios.edu/ehost/ebookviewer/ebook/bmxlYmtfXzIyOTMxMDlfX0FO0?sid=3be3e61a-003b-4171-9b17-ba1a469… 5/9

L et’s take a moment to illustrate how we can call bullshit on a fancy algorithm without ever
delving into the details of how it works, i.e., without opening the black box.

In late 2016, engineering researchers Xiaolin Wu and Xi Zhang submitted an article titled
“Automated Inference on Criminality Using Face Images” to a widely used online repository
of research papers known as the arXiv. In their article, Wu and Zhang explore the use of
machine learning to detect features of the human face that are associated with “criminality.”
They claim that their algorithm can use simple headshots to distinguish criminals from
noncriminals with high accuracy. If this strikes you as frighteningly close to Philip K. Dick’s
Precrime police in Minority Report, and to other dystopian science fiction, you’re not alone.
The media thought so too. A number of technology-focused press outlets picked up on the
story and explored the algorithm’s ethical implications. If an algorithm could really detect
criminality from the structure of a person’s face, we would face an enormous ethical
challenge. How would we have to adjust our notions of innocence and guilt if we could
identify people as criminals even before they committed a crime?

The notion that criminals are betrayed by their physiognomy is not a new one. In the
nineteenth century, an Italian doctor named Cesare Lombroso studied the anatomy of
hundreds of criminals. His aim was to develop a scientific theory of criminality. He proposed
that people were born to be criminals or to be upstanding citizens. Born criminals, he
postulated, exhibit different psychological drives and physical features. Lombroso saw these
features as hearkening back to our subhuman evolutionary past. He was particularly
interested in what could be learned from faces. In his view, the shape of the jaw, the slope of
the forehead, the size of the eyes, and the structure of the ear all contained important clues
about a man’s moral composition. Below, a figure from Cesare Lombroso’s 1876 book
Criminal Man.

45

6/27/2021 Chapter 3: The Nature of Bullshit, Calling Bullshit

https://web-b-ebscohost-com.ezproxy.losrios.edu/ehost/ebookviewer/ebook/bmxlYmtfXzIyOTMxMDlfX0FO0?sid=3be3e61a-003b-4171-9b17-ba1a469… 6/9

Lombroso was wrong. None of his theories linking anatomy to moral character have a
sound scientific basis. His ideas—many of which wrapped racist ideas of the time in a thin
veneer of scientific language—were debunked in the first half of the twentieth century and
disappeared from the field of criminology.

But in the 2016 arXiv paper, Wu and Zhang revisit Lombroso’s program. Essentially, they
aim to determine whether advanced computer vision can reveal subtle cues and patterns that
Lombroso and his followers might have missed. To test this hypothesis, the authors use
machine learning algorithms to determine what features of the human face are associated
with “criminality.” Wu and Zhang claim that based on a simple headshot, their programs can
distinguish criminal from noncriminal faces with nearly 90 percent accuracy. Moreover, they
argue that their computer algorithms are free from the myriad biases and prejudices that
cloud human judgment:

Unlike a human examiner/judge, a computer vision algorithm or classifier has
absolutely no subjective baggages [sic], having no emotions, no biases whatsoever due
to past experience, race, religion, political doctrine, gender, age, etc., no mental
fatigue, no preconditioning of a bad sleep or meal. The automated inference on
criminality eliminates the variable of meta-accuracy (the competence of the human
judge/examiner) all together.

46

6/27/2021 Chapter 3: The Nature of Bullshit, Calling Bullshit

https://web-b-ebscohost-com.ezproxy.losrios.edu/ehost/ebookviewer/ebook/bmxlYmtfXzIyOTMxMDlfX0FO0?sid=3be3e61a-003b-4171-9b17-ba1a469… 7/9

Let’s look at all of this in light of our black box schema. The machine learning algorithms
compose the black box. Most readers will not have the expertise to dig into the fine workings
of these algorithms. Even those who have the requisite background will be stymied by the
limited description of the methods in the paper. Then there are the “training sets”—the
images used to teach the algorithm how to distinguish a criminal from a noncriminal face.
These are the data that are fed into the black box. And finally, there are the facial features that
the algorithm predicts to be associated with criminality. These are the results that emerge
from the black box.

To understand the paper, we need to look at the training set. A machine learning
algorithm can be only as good as the training data that it is provided. Wu and Zhang collected
over 1,800 photos of Chinese men aged 18 to 55, with no distinguishing facial hair, scars, or
tattoos. About 1,100 of these were noncriminals. Their photographs were taken from a variety
of sources on the World Wide Web, including job-based social networking sites and staff
listings from professional firms. Just over 700 of the subjects were convicted criminals. Their
photos were provided by police departments and taken from official pieces of identification,
not from mugshots.

We see two massive problems. The first is that the images of noncriminals were selected to
cast the individuals in a positive light. By contrast, the images from the set of criminals are
official ID photographs. While it is unclear exactly what this means, it’s safe to guess that
these have been selected neither by the person depicted, nor with the aim of casting him in a
favorable light. Thank goodness no one judges our characters based upon our driver’s license
photos!

A second source of bias is that the authors are using photographs of convicted criminals. If
there are facial differences between the two groups, we won’t know whether these differences
are associated with committing crimes or with being convicted. Indeed, appearance seems to
matter for convictions. A recent study reports that in the US, unattractive individuals are
more likely to be found guilty in jury trials than their attractive peers.*2 Thus while the
authors claim that their algorithm is free of human biases, it could be picking up on nothing
but these biases.

Having identified some potential problems with the data that go into the black box, we
turn to the black box’s output. As we mentioned, the authors find that their algorithm can
classify criminal faces within their data set with 90 percent accuracy. What are the facial
features that it uses to discriminate? The algorithm finds that criminals have shorter
distances between the inner corners of the eyes, smaller angles θ between the nose and the
corners of the mouth, and higher curvature ρ to the upper lip.

47

6/27/2021 Chapter 3: The Nature of Bullshit, Calling Bullshit

https://web-b-ebscohost-com.ezproxy.losrios.edu/ehost/ebookviewer/ebook/bmxlYmtfXzIyOTMxMDlfX0FO0?sid=3be3e61a-003b-4171-9b17-ba1a469… 8/9

Why would this possibly be?

There’s a glaringly obvious explanation for the nose-mouth angle and the lip curvature. As
one smiles, the corners of the mouth spread out and the upper lip straightens. Try it yourself
in the mirror.

If you look at the original research paper, you can see six example images from the
training set. The criminals are frowning or scowling. The noncriminals are faintly smiling.
Now we have an alternative—and far more plausible—hypothesis for the authors’ findings.
There are not important differences in facial structure between criminals and noncriminals.
Rather, noncriminals are smiling in their professional headshots, whereas criminals are not
smiling in their government ID photographs. It appears that the authors have confused innate
facial features with labile facial expressions. If so, their claims about detecting criminality are
bullshit. They have not invented a criminality detector; they have invented a smile detector.

We can see further evidence of this in the output of their black box. To illustrate the
purported facial differences between criminals and noncriminals, the authors produced
composite images by aggregating features from each group. The criminal composites are
frowning, whereas the noncriminal composites are smiling. This supports our hypothesis that
the machine learning algorithm is picking up on situation-dependent facial expressions
(whether a person is smiling or not) rather than underlying facial structure.

You might find yourself thinking that even without opening the black box, this type of
analysis takes considerable time and focus. That’s true. But fortunately, some claims should
more readily trigger our bullshit detectors than others. In particular, extraordinary claims
require extraordinary evidence. The authors of this paper make the extraordinary claim that
facial structure reveals criminal tendencies. Here we see that their findings can be explained
by a much more reasonable hypothesis: People are more likely to be smiling in professional
headshots than in government ID photographs.

Notice that we established all of this without opening the black box. We didn’t have to look
at the details of the machine learning algorithms at all, because the problem didn’t arise there.
A machine learning algorithm is only as good as its training data, and these training data are
fundamentally flawed. As is often the case, one does not need technical expertise in machine
learning to call bullshit. A nonspecialist can do so by thinking carefully about what any
generic learning system would conclude from the same data. The algorithm in this paper is
not picking up some underlying physical structures associated with criminality. And it doesn’t
look like we have to worry about the ethical minefield of precrime just yet.

48

6/27/2021 Chapter 3: The Nature of Bullshit, Calling Bullshit

https://web-b-ebscohost-com.ezproxy.losrios.edu/ehost/ebookviewer/ebook/bmxlYmtfXzIyOTMxMDlfX0FO0?sid=3be3e61a-003b-4171-9b17-ba1a469… 9/9

*1 For Latour, aspects of technology or experimental procedure are “black-boxed” when they are so

thoroughly accepted by the relevant scientific community that they become generally agreed upon

standards and are no longer open to question. In Latour’s Pandora’s Hope: Essays on the Reality of

Science, he explains: “When a machine runs efficiently, when a matter of fact is settled, one need focus

only on its inputs and outputs and not on its internal complexity. Thus, paradoxically, the more science

and technology succeed, the more opaque and obscure they become.”

Here our analogy begins to break down somewhat. For the purposes of thinking about quantitative

bullshit, consider a technique to be black-boxed once it requires expertise beyond that of the typical

reader, irrespective of whether it is accepted by the scientific community.

*2 While the Chinese criminal system is structured differently from that of the US and jury trials are

rarer, the judges and occasional jurors in Chinese trials may suffer from similar biases.

Place your order
(550 words)

Approximate price: $22

Calculate the price of your order

550 words
We'll send you the first draft for approval by September 11, 2018 at 10:52 AM
Total price:
$26
The price is based on these factors:
Academic level
Number of pages
Urgency
Basic features
  • Free title page and bibliography
  • Unlimited revisions
  • Plagiarism-free guarantee
  • Money-back guarantee
  • 24/7 support
On-demand options
  • Writer’s samples
  • Part-by-part delivery
  • Overnight delivery
  • Copies of used sources
  • Expert Proofreading
Paper format
  • 275 words per page
  • 12 pt Arial/Times New Roman
  • Double line spacing
  • Any citation style (APA, MLA, Chicago/Turabian, Harvard)

Our guarantees

Delivering a high-quality product at a reasonable price is not enough anymore.
That’s why we have developed 5 beneficial guarantees that will make your experience with our service enjoyable, easy, and safe.

Money-back guarantee

You have to be 100% sure of the quality of your product to give a money-back guarantee. This describes us perfectly. Make sure that this guarantee is totally transparent.

Read more

Zero-plagiarism guarantee

Each paper is composed from scratch, according to your instructions. It is then checked by our plagiarism-detection software. There is no gap where plagiarism could squeeze in.

Read more

Free-revision policy

Thanks to our free revisions, there is no way for you to be unsatisfied. We will work on your paper until you are completely happy with the result.

Read more

Privacy policy

Your email is safe, as we store it according to international data protection rules. Your bank details are secure, as we use only reliable payment systems.

Read more

Fair-cooperation guarantee

By sending us your money, you buy the service we provide. Check out our terms and conditions if you prefer business talks to be laid out in official language.

Read more

Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code HAPPY